Wednesday, December 5, 2018

The Retention of Skills


“We don’t practice until we get it right. We practice until we can’t get it wrong,” is a phrase that I had drilled into my head as a high school athlete. My basketball coach shouted it to us at least once per week. It seems that after reading the article from Lee, Swanson, and Hall (1991) that a more appropriate phrase would have been “We don’t practice until we get it right, but we practice until we think you have all of the skills at your disposal to critically think your way through challenging situations and respond accordingly,” but that just doesn’t quite have the same ring to it.
According to Magill and Anderson (2017) the discussion about the retention of skill is essentially a debate between the application of constant practice and practice variability. Constant practice can be defined as the practice of only one variation of a specific skill while practice variability is defined as the practicing several variations of a skill (Magill & Anderson, 2017). There is a significant amount of evidence to support the efficacy of implementing a more variable approach to teaching motor skills as opposed to the blocked practice plans that most of us experienced as young athletes. Gentile’s learning stages model emphasizes a learner’s need to experience variations of regulatory and nonregulatory context characteristics, Schmidt’s schema theory has shown that the amount of movement variability a learner experiences during practice is key to predicting success in future performance, and the dynamic systems theory stresses the learner’s need to explore the perceptual motor workspace to discover the optimal solution to a motor skill problem (Magill & Anderson, 2017).
A variable that I found most interesting is how important performance error, especially when it occurs in a variable practice setting, is to skill learning (Magill & Anderson, 2017). There is evidence to support that more performance error during practice, especially during the learning stage, can be better than less error if we are trying to improve a specific motor skill (Magill & Anderson, 2017). This evidence has great implications for practitioners because it allows them to be more aggressive when trying to teach athletes or clients new skills so long as it doesn’t put the athlete or client in harm’s way.
Williams and Hodges (2005) have expressed the importance of performance error during practice as well. They suggest that this can be best facilitated by implementing random practice schedules and subsequently higher levels of contextual interference (Williams & Hodges, 2005). It is assumed that random practice, accompanied by higher contextual interference, and littered with performance errors is most beneficial to long-term skill retention because it either encourages the performer to undertake more elaborate and distinctive processing from one trial to the next or allows them to forget and reconstruct an action plan each time a skill is performed (Williams & Hodges, 2005) These hypotheses are known as the elaboration hypothesis and the action plan reconstruction hypothesis, respectively (Williams & Hodges, 2005).
I was coached by a generation that emphasized repetitious practice of the fundamentals in the hopes that it would make us better performers. It’s agonizing to see that there was research available long before I was competing in high school sports that refutes the hypothesis that practice has to be repetitive and must directly mimic skills that will be used in the context of competition. Though repetition is certainly a variable that is necessary to motor improvement, there is so much more to motor skill acquisition and retention. One must consider the cognitive processes involved if motor skills are to cross over to the competitive landscape (Lee, Swanson, & Hall, 1991). No two experiences in competition are ever the same so it’s important that athletes are equipped with the skills they need to develop their own strategies and make their own decisions and evaluations related to action. The evidence is clear that the most efficacious way to do this is through randomly organized practice. I love the Jacoby analogy in the Lee, Swanson, and Hall (1991) article because it suggests that we should be training for the ability to solve complex problems not simply retrieve answers from our memory. This analogy should also allow all coaches to audit their methods effectively. Our goal shouldn’t be to teach athletes specific actions that they can reenact whenever they encounter a recognizable scenario, but provide them with a large variety of diverse information and force them to organize it and use it effectively in their own unique manner to achieve performance success.
References
Lee, T.D., Swanson, L.R., Hall, A.L. (2014) What is repeated in repetition? Effects of practice conditions on motor skill acquisition. Journal of the American Physical Therapy Association, 71, 150-156.

Magill, R. A., & Anderson, D. I. (2017). Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications
(11th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Williams, M., & Hodges, N. J. (2005) Practice, Instruction and Skill Acquisition in Soccer:
Challenging Tradition. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23(6), 637-650.